Monthly Archives: May 2012

Symptom or syndrome ?

Parental Alienation syndrome (PAS)

What is PAS ?

New types of psychiatric disorders among children were first noted in the 1970s as adjudications in child-custody disputes rose in both number and intensity. Internationally, child contact (custody arrangement), after parental separation or divorce has become a highly contentious issue as countries drift towards unconsciously harmonising their divorce and custody regimes.

By 1985 these disorders were so noticeable and so different as to be able to be categorised and defined by Richard Gardner.

Gardner defined PAS as:

  • “a disorder that arises primarily in the context of child-custody disputes. Its primary manifestation is the child’s campaign of denigration against a parent, a campaign that has no justification. It results from the combination of a programming (brainwashing) parent’s indoctrination and the child’s own contributions to the vilification of the targeted parent.”

He went on to emphasis the point that if “true parental abuse and/or neglect is present” and the child’s animosity is justified, PAS would not be an appropriate explanation for the children’s feelings.

As more psychiatric professionals have examined the matter the definition has undergone various revisions. The 1987 definition of Parental Alienation (for example by Douglas Darnall, PhD 1997), is different from Dr. Gardner’s original definition of PAS in 1987:

  • “a disturbance in which children are preoccupied with deprecation and criticism of a parent-denigration that is unjustified and/or exaggerated.”

A broader view of parental alienation (PA), would be “any constellation of behaviours, whether conscious or unconscious, thatcould evoke a disturbance in the relationship between a child and the other parent.”

Note here the simple difference in wording which caused a storm in academia in the 1990s and into 2000s re: parental alienation (PA) vs Parental Alienation Syndrome (PAS).

The idea that PAS was a phenomenon damaging to children and that  it was commonly found in divorcing situations was then so utterly and universally refuted by academics that it makes one question whether PAS really does exist or, whether vested interested cannot afford to recognise it. Inevitably, views became not only politicised but polarised.

In Germany, psychologist Kodjoe (1998; 2001) drew attention to studies of Clawar & Rivlin (1991) in support of PAS.

The following decades have seen a reversal of that attitude and recognition of its existence.[1]

For all intents and purposes they are the same but a huge resistance was mounted in learned journals objecting to it being given ‘syndrome’ status.

‘Malicious Mother Syndrome’ and ‘Implacable Hostility’ by the parent-with-custody are also alternative terms used for the same or similar situations.

PAS involves the child as an active participant inflicting the discomfort not merely observing it as in PA.

As parents and fathers all this is immaterial – it must be the welfare of the child thatshould over ride petty squabbles. Whether it is the vilification of the targeted parent by the coached child (PAS), or the child observing by the child of the vilification of the targeted parent (PA)both situations qualify as ‘child abuse.’

There is a link between PAS and time spent with the second parent – usually the father. Contact or visiting time can either be welcomed by the parent-with-care (usually the mother) or rejected and denied. The latter leading to the sabotaging of contact time with arguments and deterioration in parental communications then ensues. Where a child’s time with the other parent (usually the father) is welcomed and encouraged by the mother, PAS seems not to occur.

In the Netherlands, Spruijt, at Utrecht University, had by 2004 accepted that:

  • “Nobody disputes the phenomenon of parental alienation in the context of conflicts about custody and visitation.

In Holland about 20% of children do not have any contact with their non-resident parent after parental divorce (Spruijt). [2]

  • “There are often many reasons underlying the broken contact, but one might well be the process of parental alienation, when the child denigrates and excludes the non-resident parent.”

Spruijt conducted two studies (of 138 respondents) in 2004 among a). divorce ‘experts’ and b). divorced, non-resident parents. Not surprisingly, 58% thought PAS either did not, or hardly ever, occurred in the Netherlands, and 42% thought it did. The extent of parental alienation was classified as mild (33%) or moderate (in 9% of cases).

However, Gardner and Dr Douglas Darnall list not 2 but 3 types of parental alienation syndrome and 3 categories of ‘parental alienators’, severe; moderate; and mild.

The three types of parental alienation syndrome which, though dealt with more fully in the Annex, can be summarised as:

1. The Campaign of Denigration:

Typically the child is obsessed with “hatred” of a parent, yet when other prompts are used displays many tender and loving feelings felt toward the allegedly despised parent. These children speak of the hated parent with every vilification and profanity in their vocabulary—without embarrassment or guilt.

With only a minimal amount of prompting by a lawyer or mental health professional, the child’s list has the quality of a litany and is almost word perfect whenever asked.

2. The Weak, Frivolous, or Absurd Rationalizations for the Deprecation:

Typically, a PAS child will provide irrational and often ludicrous justifications for their alienation from the ‘target’ parent. The child may justify the alienation with memories of minor disagreements or disappointments experienced many years ago which they blame on the victimised parent. These are usually trivial and are experiences that most children quickly forget, for example, “He [she] always used to speak very loud when he told me to brush my teeth.”

3. Lack of Ambivalence:

We know from our own ‘life experiences’ that nothing is ever wholly black or wholly white; rather it is grey or a mixture of both black and white. All human relationships display this ambivalence and parent-child relationships are no exception. Yet children who have undergone ‘coaching’ by an alienating parent are unable to list good attributes of the parent whom they are vilifying. This is a serious abnormality in the psychotically make up of the child and is indicative of a mild form of what used to be termed “brain washing.”

Even if the victimized (targeted) parent has been deeply dedicated to the child’s upbringing, with strong bonds forged over many years, the PAS child may not be able to think of one single thing she (he) ever liked about the targeted parent.

There are types of ‘parental alienators’, Naïve Alienators; Active Alienators; Obsessed Alienators.

A. The Naïve Alienator

Naïve Alienator: Most divorced parents have moments or occasions when they are naive alienators.

  • “Tell your father thathe has more money than I do, so let him buy your soccer boots.”

These parents mean well and recognise the importance of the children having a healthy relationship with the other parent. They encourage the relationship between the children and the other parent and their family and they rarely have to return to court because of problems with visits.

The characteristics of naïve alienators are:

  • a). Their ability to separate in their minds the children’s needs from their own.
  • b). They recognise the importance for the children to spend time with the other parent so they can build a mutually loving relationship.
  • c). They avoid making the other parent a target for their hurt and loss.

B. The Active Alienator

Most parents returning to court over problems with visitation (contact / access) are ‘active alienators.’ These parents mean well and believe thatthe children should have a healthy relationship with the other parent. The problem they have is with controlling their frustration, and containing their bitterness or hurt (often in hindsight over the divorce).

After a “trigger” event active alienators will lash out in a way thatcauses or reinforces alienation against the targeted parent. For example:

  • “I don’t want you to tell your father thatI earned this extra money. The miser will take it from his child support check thatwill keep us from going toDisneyworld. You remember he’s done this before when we wanted to go to Grandma’s for Christmas.”

After regaining control, the parent will usually feel guilty or bad about whatthey did and back off from their alienating tactics.

Oscillating between impulsively alienating and then repairing the damage done to the child is the trademark of the active alienator.

The characteristics of active alienators are:

  • a). Lashing out at the other parent in front of the children. Their problem has more to do with loss of self-control when they are upset than with a sinister motivation.
  • b). After calming down, active alienators realize thatthey were wrong. They usually try to repair any damage or hurt to the children. During the making up, such parents can be very comforting and supportive of the child’s feelings.
  • c). Like naïve alienators, they are able to differentiate between their needs and those of the children by supporting the children’s desire to have a relationship with the other parent.

C. The Obsessed Alienator

As the title indicates obsessive alienators are dangerous. The obsessed alienator is a parent, (or sometimes a grandparent), with a point to prove and feel they are championing a ‘cause.’

They work actively and persistently to alienate the child from its other targeted parent. For the alienator’s campaign to work the children’s personalities and beliefs have to be ruthlessly enmeshed in the obsessed alienator’s distorted view of the world.

Certain phrases characterise the Obsessive alienator:

  • “I love my children. If the court can’t protect them from their abusive father, I will.
  • Even though he’s never abused the children, I know it’s a matter of time.
  • The children are frightened of their father.
  • If they don’t want to see him, I’m not going to force them.
  • They are old enough to make up their own minds.”

The process takes time and it is one that the child, especially the young, are completely helpless to recognise and combat. It usually begins well before the divorce has begun but not yet ‘finalised’; the obsessed parent is angry, bitter or feels betrayed by the other parent;  things aren’t working out as well as she had hoped.

The characteristics of obsessed alienators are:

  • They are obsessed with destroying the children’s relationship with the targeted parent.
  • They have succeeded in enmeshing the children’s personalities with their own.
  • The children will ‘parrot’ the obsessed alienator’s views rather than express their own feelings from personal experience with the other parent.
  • Often the children cannot tell you the reasons for their feelings.
  • The obsessed alienator believes in a higher cause, protecting the childrenatall cost.
  • The court’s authority does not intimidate them.
  • Their beliefs sometimes becoming delusional and irrational. No one, especially the court, can convince obsessed alienators that they are wrong. Anyone who tries instantly becomes ‘the enemy.’
  • They have an unquenchable anger because they believe thatthe targeted parent has victimized them and whatever they do to protect the children is justified.
  • They have a desire for the court to punish the other parent with court orders thatwould interfere or block the targeted parent from seeing the children. This confirms in the obsessed alienator’s mind thathe or she was right all the time.
  • The obsessed alienator will never want to read whatis on these pages because it just makes them angrier.

Parental alienation syndrome (PAS) in the Netherlands

Ed Spruijt, PhD, is associate professor in the Department of Child and Adolescent Studies,Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands.

Abstract

In the Netherlands, about 20% of children do not have any contact with their non-resident parent after parental divorce. There are often many reasons underlying the broken contact, but one might well be the process of parental alienation, when the child denigrates and excludes the non-resident parent. This article presents the results of two studies conducted amongst divorce experts and divorced, non-resident parents. A total of 138 respondents co-operated in our studies.

Of the respondents, 58% thought PAS either does not, or hardly, occur in theNetherlands, and 42% thought it does.

The extent of parental alienation was classified as mild (33%) or moderate (9%). From our factor analysis, it became clear thatGardner’s classification of eight separate symptoms of parental alienation was not evident in our research data. We were able to distinguish four separate aspects: two of them concerning alienation due to the resident parent and two concerning alienation due to the child. Our results underpin the importance of mediation, since it seemed that PAS occurred significantly more often when decisions with relation to the children were not taken together by the parents but were determined in court. We consider that compulsory mediation and better communication during divorce would prevent many cases of PAS.

  • The Judiciary’s Role in the Etiology, Symptom Development, and Treatment of the Parental Alienation Syndrome (PAS)

Richard A. Gardner, MD

(circa 2002)

The parental alienation syndrome (PAS) is a psychiatric disorder that arises in the course of child-custody disputes adjudicated in the context of adversarial proceedings. This article describes the central role that such proceedings have had in the development of this relatively new disorder.

Our legal system does not stand alone in having produced this disorder; litigating parents as well as their children have played an important contributory role. It is the purpose of this article to focus on the judiciary’s role in the etiology, development of symptoms, and treatment of the parental alienation syndrome. It is the author’s hope thatincreasing recognition of the PAS by the judiciary will enhance its ability to make prudent decisions in child-custody disputes in which the children have developed manifestations of this now widespread disorder.

Since the 1970s, we have witnessed a burgeoning of child-custody disputes unparalleled in history. This increase has primarily been the result of two recent developments in the realm of child-custody litigation, namely, the replacement of the tender-years presumption with the best-interests-of-the-child presumption and the increasing popularity of the joint-custodial concept. Under the tender-years presumption, the assumption was made that mothers, by virtue of the fact that they are female, are intrinsically superior to men as child rearers. Accordingly, the father had to provide the court with compelling evidence of serious maternal deficiencies before the court would even consider assigning primary custodial status to the father. Under its replacement, the best-interests-of-the-child presumption, the courts were instructed to ignore gender when adjudicating child-custody disputes and evaluate only parenting capacity, especially factors that related to the best interests of the child. This change resulted in a burgeoning of custody litigation as fathers found themselves with a greater opportunity to gain primary custodial status. Soon thereafter the joint-custodial concept came into vogue, eroding even further the time that custodial mothers were given with their children. Again, this change also brought about an increase and intensification of child-custody litigation

E N D  

 


[1] For instance, in the UK Sturge & Glaser’s report to the Children Act Sub-Committee (CASC) in 2002 admitted “alienation” but dismissed it as not being entered into the DSM (theUS based psychological manual).

[2]Parental alienation syndrome (PAS) in the Netherlands”, associate professor in the Department of Child and Adolescent Studies, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands 2004.

P.A.S. comes of age ?

by Robert Whiston,   Feb 2012
 
 
An overview of the inter-related subjects of sole mother custody, PAS, shared parenting and best ‘outcomes’ for children
 
THE  ARTICLE  BELOW  FORMED THE BASIS OF  A PAPER  DRAFTED  FOR  PRESENTATION  BY THE ‘PLATFORM  FOR  EUROPEAN  FATHERS’ (PEF) FOR  A  SEMINAR  ON  ‘PARENTAL ALIENATION’   HELD  IN  PORTUGAL  (MARCH  22nd – 23rd  2012).
 
Everywhere one looks the old ways are being questioned and our once familiar ‘social fabric’ is, in many instances being torn to shreds – from the Face-book revolutions of North Africa to the ‘Occupy Wall St’ campaigners in New York it is now the western world  that is in upheaval.

Gov’t and policy makers will, more than ever, have to stay ahead of the game or face rebuff or even overthrow – Eastern Europe failed to do that 20 years ago.

Yes, it has taken a number of years but the passing of the millennium (2000) has finally proven to be the watershed in human history that was long predicted.

In the last 10 years we can clearly see that it is now the age of the citizen.

The era of personal rights has now fully blossomed. No longer is the common man a mere cog in the political process – he is the political process ; he is the the agent for change.

No longer are citizens content to be told what is good for them – patronising voters no longer works.

Ordinary citizens will not tolerate being told what they can and can’t have in this ‘second generation’ of freedom.

The ‘Stop Online Piracy Act’ (SOPA), which would have affected the humble enjoyment millions of ‘ordinary citizens’ has been rejected by them and citizen power has stopped the legislation in its tracks. The same fate has befallen ‘PIPA’ (Protect Intellectual Property Act). Both were direct assaults by corporate greed on the rights and freedoms of the individual.

This has meant that all across Europe the child custody debate has moved to the top of the political agenda. The battle lines are essentially the stark choice between remaining with mother-only-custody of the child versus shared parenting where both parents are participants in child custody and care.

Much is at stake – not just for old-school-feminists, who support the former failed regime, and fathers, who support the latter, but for children and whether the balanced, healthy society we all seek will become a reality. This is a clash that must be won.

It cannot become, as American author Warren Farrell famously put it, an undeclared war won on a battlefield where only one side turned up.

The overiding question today is whether we want children in the post-divorce scenario to grow up to be dysfunctional and a liability and a burden on the state, or as glistening jewels in society’s crown ?

After 30 years of feigning deafness, politicians across Europe are acknowledging the contributions and efforts fathers can and should be allowed to ‘socialise’ to young children so that in spite of  the setbacks that divorce inevitably brings, children will become the glistening jewels society seeks and values.

This cannot be done under the present regime of mother-only-custody found in most European countries. A fact that is being increasingly recognised in many quarters. For instance, the present British government has stated in 2012 that:

  • “Our vision is to establish that, under normal circumstances, a child will have relationship with both his and her parents, regardless of their relationship with each other.”

Britain of 2012 is not alone in Europe in recognising it is time to give children and father’s legal rights after divorce. Sweden and Belgium have enacted notable reforming legislation.

Traditionally, the right in politics has tried to support the family by championing financial incentives and fighting a cultural battle against those who don’t conform.

For its part the traditional left in politics has promised freer, less permanent, more liberal matrimonial ties and encouraged alternative life styles based on human liberties and sexual orientation.

Both left and right have ‘won’ particular battles – but in the process have lost the war. Ther have been a series of Pyrrhic Victories. Yes, the battle for radical social reform has been won but it has been at such a cost and so socially disruptive that the victory is tantamount to a defeat.

The monetary cost of reform caused both by both left and right political reforms (but mostly left-wing) is large, however, the social cost are larger and it is only now that a realisation is growing that some alleged ‘social reforms’ are actually counter-productive.

The collateral damage caused by a system of divorce and custody that suits no one, is a system that costs everyone dearly. Governments in the EU spend billion of Euros every year; the cost of legal cases, salaries of judges; court staff; state benefit payments to spouses; the compensation required when losing the matrimonial home; the legal fees paid; the cost of lost production are also ‘time’ costs that can never be recovered. The downstream or “outcome” costs comprise criminality and anti-social behavior, drug addiction and a high incidence of ‘run-aways’ from home; and for girls, higher probability of teenager pregnancies. And it is these immutable, measuarable, objective “outcomes” that are at last winning the political battle.

Impact of the ‘state wage’

The costs of our present failed system thus racks up all these costs as each year goes by.

Once the divorce and custody dispute are over there is the on-going cost to the welfare state payments made to single mothers and their children who are wholly dependent on a “state wage.” 

Those children need not only food, clothing, shoes and shelter but medicines, visits to doctors, hospital treatment, dentists, opticians, medication and education (to name but a few).

All those bills the tax-payer has to pay and for no fault of his own, his standard of living – and that of his wife and children – declines.

Is it any wonder that a few years ago – long before the present economic recession – countries like New Zealand had to abandon all military spending and had to sell-off their entire Air Force and most of their Navy ? So great had been their well-meant social packages and ‘wage’ compensation benefits that they are now an undefended country. ‘Defence of the Realm’ as a concept and as a practical reality, are no longer possible.

The state has too often seen itself as a better parent then the biological parent. This was actually stated by one Labour minister (Alan Johnson MP ?). Now the state is finally realising it cannot create ‘happy families’ or broker amicable break-ups.

Michael Gove MP, minister for education and schools, recognised that the state was a very blunt and damaging instument of social change when he said at an IPPR conference (a think-tank) in the Aug 2008:

  • “But if children are having decent, loving parents pushed out of their lives, we owe it to them to change the system that lets this happen.”

This has been the problem all across Europe – loving parents are being pushed out of their children’s lives for no good reason (in the majority of cases).

One British Government official said this year that they wanted to remove any “in-built legal bias against the father or the mother” in the law.

The following 3 statements are, in our view, central and pivotal to the nature of reform:

  1. “[ the issue ] is about the children. Both parents should have a full and continuing role in a child’s life after they separate.
  2. “Where there are no significant welfare issues, we would want to see this principle reinforced through law. We will make a legislative statement emphasising the importance of children having an ongoing relationship with both their parents at separation, when it is safe and in the child’s best interests.
  3. “In all of this, the most important thing remains the principle that the child’s welfare is the paramount consideration and this must not be diluted.”

This is no more and no less than what is required and requested by fathers groups in every land across Europe and North America.

The reason why this problem appears so widely spread is because of changes in legislation that a host of countries copied one another in the decade’s after 1960. This is not unique occurrence; one only has to look at divorce reform, meaning easier divorces and domestic violence legislations to see that they differ little in content from one nation to another. In the case of divorce reform the year of implementation across a gamut of countries looks ominously like orchestration – it all happened between the years 1968 – 1971.

Opponents of Shared Parenting are happy to confuse two different concepts namely that of strict/rigid allocations of “equal time” that a child spends with each parent, with Shared Parenting which gives both parents reasonable amounts of time with their child depending on their working hours and domestic load. Opponents of Shared Parenting have even stooped to confusing in the public’s mind sole-mother custody – where residence is given to the mother and the father is only allowed to visit occasionally –  and dubbed it shared parenting.

In the past the terms Equal Parenting and Shared Parenting have sometimes been synonymous, with different countries preferring to use one term in favour of the other while all meaning the same arrangements. That is now slowly changing to one where

Equal Parenting starts from a premise of equal parenting time subject to it being practical and parents agreeing.

Shared Parenting starts from the premise that a father’s contact or visitation with his child should be more generous than the alternate weekend and occasional mid-week rendevous of a few hours. The amount of time totals no more than 14% per annum of a childs life at present and should, subject to mutual agreement be closer to 30/70, or 40/60 time split depending on work, family and home commitments.

At present the normal ‘contact’ time awarded to fathers in the majority of cases is alternating weekend visiting, i.e. once a fortnight, and possibly a mid-week visit.

On the barest basis of solely alternating weekend visiting, a father would see his child for 14% of the year (2 x daytime visits, limited to less than 12 hours and no overnight ‘sleepover’ [51/365 =  14%]).

Sometimes a patient father can gradually work towards being given an overnight ‘sleepover’ concession. This would raise his time allocation to 19% (3 days per fortnight [72/365 = 19%])

Were a father be allowed to see his child for 3 days every week (and not fortnightly) the time spent would increase still further to 39% (144 days = 39%).

However, all these figures can be a misleading since the child has to be returned as night and so it could be argued the time a father spends with his child is only half a day since the child returns at night to the mother’s residence. For this reason the regime is called sole mother custody for the mother has the child for 365 days of the year and there is no sharing.

School holiday periods can provide additional time to be spent with fathers; however, since this is also prime opportunity for maternal sabotage it is not included in the above calculations. Shared parenting would see mothers lose the 365 day monopoly and the child would be shared, perhaps 100 days and nights for the fathers and 200 for the mother.

The present British Government now accepts – where the previous Labour government did not that:

  • “Quite clearly, ordinary living and working arrangements make an equal division impossible, and undesirable, in all but a small minority of cases.”

This view is endorsed by a poll of readers in the Daily Telegraph (80% in favour) and reinforces polls carried out in Australia where shared parenting was introduced in 2006.

Government ministers now believe, and hopefully theattendant divorce industry will follow, that “ . ..  the courts are rarely the best place for resolving private disputes about the care of children.”

To that end a £10 million fund has been set up to encourage more parents to use mediation. Indeed, Minister Michael Gove, in the Telegraph (Feb 2012), is repoorted as believing that:

  •  “ . . .  we need to do much more to ensure that fathers play their full part in the lives of their children.”

Disingenuously, some researchers define sole custody to the mother as shared custody. This they achieve by starting from the premise thatsole custody means precisely that, i.e. mother or one parent only for 100% of the child’s time – it has never meant that, except in the case of widowhood, but we shall not go into details atthis juncture. Therefore, they must reason, any “sharing” of custody such as permitting a father to see his child for a number of hours at weekends or during the week is ‘shared parenting’. They then report the findings of parental relationships they define as shared parenting. Unsurprisingly they find:

  • “Boys in rigidly sustained shared care were most likely to have Hyperactivity/Inattention scores in the clinical/borderline range.” – Jennifer McIntosh.
  • “Fathers with shared care arrangements were the most satisfied with the arrangement, despite reporting higher levels of on-going conflict about parenting and poorer dispute management.” – Jennifer McIntosh.

Either Jennifer McIntosh has not examined the situation carefully enough or is deliberately misconstruing what she has been told. No one is asking for ‘rigidly’ imposed shared care orders for children. Her “shared care arrangements” definition it later transpires is what everyone else calls sole mother custody, i.e. fathers having only minimal contact at weekends.

What Jennifer McIntosh and her ilk are measuring in many instances are actually sole mother custody awards and perversely their conclusions coincide with the findings of fathers’ groups, namely, that children brought up in households headed by single mothers and no fathers display a variety of pathologies.

Where father are only allowed to visit for a few hours per week / per day children will display a). high hyperactivity / inattention and, b). higher levels of on-going conflict about parenting and more disputes.[1]

That is precisely our argument.

In years gone by, when government thought it knew best, government policies went out of their way to prevent this happening.

Simple matters like recording the fathers name on the child’s birth certificate were left to fall into disuse thus undermining the important role fathers can play in a child’s sense of identity.

Cultural signals and social norms do influence behaviour and the costs of relationship breakdown, of children left fatherless, of men behaving badly, are borne by us all.

Politicians are forever trying to ‘signal’ to the markets their displeasure at certain actions or to prevent certain decisions yet seem genuinely surprise that their policies and signals have shut out fathers from children’s lives.

But when the state offers incentives and rewards for relationship breakdown then those cultural signals have, for political reasons, been unmentioned. Equally, when the state has encouraged fatherless by its welfare payments policies, the cost is borne by society and the cause never debated.

Profit and wealth were for a long time after 1945 terms and concepts held in disdain. Indeed, they are not so universally approved of today but without a greater spread of profit and wealth countries like Greece will not quickly overcome her problems.

So, while all European countries try to figure out how they can pump wealth into their deflated economies, it is instructive to view the increasing numbers of fathers separated from their children in terms of wealth generation and in particular of wealth accumulation.

The following diagram explains the wealth creating capacities of men and women with both genders divided between married couples and singles:

Earning capacities 

Men

Women

Single

1.0 (G) H1

1.0 (G) H1

Married

1.7 (G) H2

0.6 / 1.02 H2

Indexed, where G = Gilder and H = Hewitt (IPPR)

                       

As wealth producers, single men and women are comparable at 1.0, but it is the married man who at 1.7 is the true ‘engine of wealth generation’ in any economy. Even if the married man’s wealth generating capacity is coupled to his wife’s, their joint capacity still exceeds that of combining two cohabiting singletons into a couple. A state full of single cohabiting persons is therefore unlikely to generate as much wealth.

The reason for this is simple – the married man has a long term ‘investment’ in society – he has an investment in his family, i.e. his children. His children are his society (see Popenoe D, Annex A). Take a man’s children away and he immediately becomes a single man again without any of that investment or lust to succeed.

An inefficient use of resources can be tolerated in economically good times but is utterly reprehensible and near-suicidal in times of economic hardship.

As long ago as the mid-1970s these distinct trends became apparent to George Gilder who was then a US Presidential advisor (see “Men and Marriage“); they were validated in 1990 by Amneus in his book “The Garbage Generation”; and the distinct categories were again endorsed in 1993 by Patricia Hewitt & Penelope Leach – perhaps unwittingly – in their IPPR paper.

The media is forever happy to depict men and fathers as all behaving badly or label them as being feckless, but where men and fathers are given the right opportunities they rise to the occasion and show their maturity the dividends are handsome.       

Those societies which help fathers play their full, and equal, role enjoy greater family stability as a result. In Sweden, the introduction of flexible parental leave helped increase the amount of time fathers spent with their young children and helped the divorce rate fall by 30%.

In Belgium and Australia, which introduced new shared parenting laws in the same year, it will only take a few more years to establish a reliable and identifiable trend – but we can predict it will have much to commend it.

It’s important that we see parenting as a shared responsibility, with fathers playing an equal and complementary role, to mothers. It is not enough to simply legislate and say that fathers now have ‘joint responsibility’ – it must be tangible; there must be meat on the bone, and have substance – not simply rhetoric.

Over the past 40 years marriage itself has changed and fatherhood within those marriages has also changed. Men are far more ‘hands on’ than they used to be.

And where relationships do founder (i.e. end in divorce), it’s important also that we ensure fathers play the fullest possible role in those circumstances. In so doing we guarantee the emotional stability so essential for the children involved for make no mistake the outcomes for children in terms of morbidity and mortality, are far better where fathers are actively engaged with their children through the growing up years. A possible by-product of this new approach will be a decline in mental health problems and hospital admissions that single mothers especially appear to suffer from.

If we want children to be those jewels in the crown then we can wait no longer for reform.

The other paths has been tried and shown to fail.

We need to take the path that the people want – and now.

In Nov 2011, Linda Nielsen, Professor of Women’s Studies, Wake Forest University, in North Carolina, USA, published a paper entitled:

“Shared Parenting After Divorce: A Review of Shared Residential Parenting Research” (ref http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10502556.2011.619913 ).

I can do no better than to quote directly from the 7 points in her preamble:-

  1. One of the most complex and compelling issues confronting policymakers, parents, and the family court system is what type of parenting plan is most beneficial for children after their parents’ divorce.
  2. How much time should children live with each parent?
  3. An increasing number of children are living with each parent at least 35% of the time in shared residential parenting families:
  4. How are these children and their parents faring?
  5. In what ways, if any, do divorced parents who share the residential parenting differ from parents whose children live almost exclusively with their mother?
  6. How stable are shared residential parenting plans?
  7. By reviewing the existing studies on shared parenting families, these questions are addressed.

What is the most beneficial living arrangement in terms of its impact on the well-being of children has been the Holy Grail of law makers and courts for many decades ? Sometimes Social Scientists have obfuscated on the issues; sometimes they have preferred not to be drawn one way or the other.

In the absence of leadership we have, on the one hand, reluctance to change and on the other, an inability to present information clearly. Clearly, in such circumstances another avenue has to be found that gives us a better insight into the impact on children.

Leaving aside the personal and political prejudices of many of those that have shaped child custody and welfare matters since 1945, it has fallen to ‘outcomes’ to be that new  avenue.

The advantage ‘outcomes’ presents to policy makers is that they carry no political baggage, no academic reputations are in jeopardy and they are objective measures. The data is based not on a small self-selecting sample but on a much larger random population which makes their accuracy (and relevance) far greater.

The paper by Linda Nielsen lists some of those ‘outcomes’ and helps show us the way; her paper builds on the work done by other modern researchers.

In the face of ‘failing’ child care policies she asks specifically;

  • “How much time should children live with each of their parents in families where both parents are fit and loving ?

Indeed, she goes on to ask:

  • “Do children benefit from living with both parents at least 35% of the time ? And if children do benefit how and why does this come about ?

For over a century it has been noted that statistically children where the father is present live longer than where the father is absent. This is called the ‘mortality rate.’

Equally, children where the father is not present statistically suffer higher ‘morbidity rates’, i.e. rates of illnesses and accidents.’ This distinction applies regardless of country.

So, firstly, do parents who share residential custody differ in significant ways from the majority of divorced parents whose children live almost exclusively with their mother ?

And secondly, why is the IQ and ‘life chances’ of children brought up with a fathers present so much higher ?

In the past 20 years these questions have become increasingly relevant for at least three reasons.

Firstly, shared residential custody is becoming more prevalent. Until recently only 5% to 7% of children lived at least one third of the time with their father.

Most children of divorce lived exclusively with their mother, spending only four or five nights a month – at most – in their father’s home (Kelly, 2007). Translated into time this equates to 14% to the father and 86% to the mother.

However, a change is clearly underway all over Europe – in the Netherlands, Denmark, and Sweden 18% to 20% of children whose parents have separated are in shared residential custody (Smyth, 2009; Spruijt & Duindam, 2010).

Belgium, which was not mentioned by Linda Nielsen, adopted in 2006 a reform for time to be allocated to fathers.

The same change is underway in Australia where shared parenting was enacted in 2006. In the USA, Arizona and in Washington State have between 30% to 50% of the children whose parents divorced in the past several years are living at least one third of the time with each parent (Ref. George, 2008; Venohr & Kaunelis, 2008). NB, the Washington State model is not true shared parenting.

In Wisconsin, a study by Melli & Brown (in 2008) revealed that 30% of the children whose parents divorced between 1996 and 2001 were in shared parenting arrangements.

Secondly, the question of shared residential custody has become increasingly relevant for the majority of people who want to live in a society of equals and of equal opportunity. In an era where young fathers are more obviously engaged with their young children why should they be excluded once a divorce occurs ?

In a variety of surveys over a number of years the public has declared its support for shared parenting. In a 2004 a poll in Massachusetts showed 85% of the voters (530,000 people) wanted shared parenting. The figures for England obtained by the Dept of Constitutional Affairs are in the 70% range. Surveys of young people, i.e. tomorrow’s parents, show the same sort of range, i.e. 70% – 80%.

Thirdly, given that the question of shared residential custody has climbed up the political agenda in a big way and is competing for the attention of legislators and politicians many of them (and the professions) need to become familiar with what it entails and the many benefits to all parties that ensue.

Regardless of regime of law – be it common law or continental statue law – the changes proposed entail a “presumption” of equal residential custody. This would mean thatwhere parents reach a mutual agreement (using a parenting plan), the time the child or children spends with each parent can be as flexible and as varied as each parent finds it comfortable.

Where parents cannot reach a mutual agreement on the kind of parenting plan they think is best for their children, the children would live equal time with each of them as long as both were “fit and loving” parents. This is often enough to break the log jam of ‘difficult parent (s)’, for both parents would soon realise that their work, social and home life would be put under strain and curtailed.

It is perhaps a reflection of our times that a book entitled “Collateral damage: the lived experiences of divorced mothers without custody” can be written and cause no ruffles.

The proportion of such divorced mothers amounts to no more than 5% -7% of all divorced mothers, yet no book has been written dealing with the collateral damage: the lived experiences of divorced fathers without custody, who represent over 90% of divorced men and an army numbering tens of millions.

In part this absence of the other side’s case is due to funding being made available for one style of survey but not another. Single mothers have long been an issue high on political agenda and this has crowded out researchers looking at the other side of the coin, single fathers and fathers without children.

The next diagram shows ‘outcomes’ for children lists 13 studies into shared parenting and the benefits ensuing. The studies cover the years 1991 to 2010. With very few exceptions all show ‘better adjustment’ in shared parenting living arrangements.

Transferring the Table has unfortunately resulted in a poor image: the 7 headings are (L to R), Study; Other; Psychological emotional;  Behavioural ; Relationship with father; Relationship with mother; Physical health. This Table (listed as Table 2), shows the ‘outcomes’ for children brought up in shared residential custody.

Only in a few studies are ‘outcomes’ for single parent custody listed as being the same as shared parenting, which perhaps reflects the design (or designer) of the study more than the true variation.

Breivik & Olweus conclude that in matters academic, drugs and alcohol there is no difference. In the study by Spruijt & Duindam, other work by Spruijt would lead one to conclude this to be in line with his personal beliefs.

Importantly, none of them show worse or bad outcomes.

All the other above studies conclude – as Bausermann did in his meta-analysis – that children are better off (better adjusted) in shared parenting living arrangements.

The opposite of ‘well adjusted’ children is mal-adjusted and this has been a phenomenon among children throughout history. Often anti-social behaviour has been a rites of passage among young males. However, there has always been someone – or a small group –that has not fitted in and for whom disruptive behaviour is not limited to a ‘phase they grow out of.’

Importantly, none of them show worse or bad outcomes.

Why is this  ?

Hasn’t the opposition to fathers being more involved in their children’s lives forever rested on the premise on the damage and negative impact they can cause ?

The answer to that is ‘yes’ it has and one then has to look for the reason why such a view was ever held – and it is not long in the finding.

It was in the 1930s that this once small group became too big to ignore. It was in that period, that the term ‘juvenile delinquent’ came to the public’s attention through American gangster and police films of the period. Children from broken homes would go on from juvenile hall to commit bigger crimes and end up in jail and ‘death row’ despite the best intentions of their probation officer.

At about the same time academics such as John Bowlby and Anna Freud began examining the effects of orphaned children and children with psychological damage.

There are always those in the academic world prepared to conduct studies to show that:

  • “some children reared in orphanages cope rather better than those reared at home.” [2]

Yet we know from the pathologies exhibited that this is simply not true. Yes, a few may achieve great heights but the majority will not.

The merging of the juvenile delinquent’ with the psychologically damaged, orphaned and/or traumatised child happened without anyone noticing their distinct pedigrees.

Around these two Bowlby and Freud gathered in Londonthe glitterati of psychoanalyses, Freud, Dann, Burlingham, Melanie Klein, Donald Winnicott, and Mary Ainsworth. [3]

Anna Freud – herself a psychologically damaged child with numerous sexual and social dysfunctions – began her work among Jewish children inVienna in the 1930s. That work was cut short by the arrival of a Nazi regime in Austria and her escape to England.

However, one client Dorothy Burlingham, followed her to London. Dorothy Burlingham believed all her 4 children needed help and for the next 40 years were be put under the psychological control of Anna Freud.

The results were not glorious. Psychologically, all the children became  mixed up and confused.

All the children suffered from alcoholism and drug abuse with several overdoses. In 1970 Robert Burlingham Jr. (Dorothy Burlingham’s only son) reportedly committed suicide aged just 55, and in 1974 a daughter ‘Mabbie’ booked an appointment with Anna Freud and then, as a statement, committed suicide in Freud’s own home. (see “Anna Freud: Part 1 – Her secret failure”, http://robertwhiston.wordpress.com/2009/09/01/0013/).

But all that was an ‘outcome’ waiting to happen – waiting to be chronicled.

Meanwhile in the 1940s Anna Freud had renewed her interest with children by focusing on those orphaned and traumatised during the Luftwaffe Blitz on London in the 1940s. This work was expanded by generous US funding at a new centre, Hampstead Nurseries, in  Hampstead, London (later called “Hampstead Child Clinic”). Still later, after 1945 she opened Bulldogs Banks and experimented with 6 very young feral Jewish children rescued from a concentration camp.

Similarly, Donald Winnicott, and Bowlby’s work revolve around disturbed evacuee children – sometimes profoundly disturbed psychologically

For 40 years Winnicott had worked at Paddington Green Children’s as a paediatrician and child psychoanalyst and during World War II he served as consultant psychiatrist to the evacuee programme.

John Bowlby is best known for his “hierarchy of attachment” theory. Briefly stated this believes that mothers are the prime and normally the only caregiver a children recognises and forms a bond with in the early months of life. This we now know is a reflection of the social norms of the time and not true science since it has been shown many times in subsequent studies (e.g. Prof Michael Rutter, Richard A. Warshak etc), that infants can and do form bonds with fathers or other close and constant relatives.

Indeed, one might say that Freud’s creation of a ‘psychological parent’ cuts the very ground from under Bowlby’s thesis about the centrality of mothers.

But it was Anna Freud and her circle that became absurdly influential in government policy circles in the decades after 1945.

The state had during World War II – even in advanced democracies – run government on the basis of a ‘command economy’ which meant it intruded at all levels. And it has to be admitted it was essential when civilisation itself was put in peril. However, like all acquired tastes it became a hard habit to break.

In the case of Britain the state had from 1940 to 1945 and again until 1948 positively encouraged divorce firstly in order to avert demoralisation among troops and their families and secondly to recognise that much had gone on contraty to vows taken. When these emergency laws were removed divorce numbers began falling back towards it pre-war levels. Five year plans and centralised strategy setting was again essential if the Marshall Aid was to be successful in the rebuilding of a shatteredEurope

Winnicott and Bowlby gave the world two enduring legacies – one was the “good-enough mother” [4] and Bowlby’s observation that:

“A bad home is better than a good institution.” [5]

Bowlby’s, 1944 famous paper “The Thieves Study” focused on maternal deprivation. He believed that the infant and its mother bond during the first five years of life were most crucial to socialisation. Disruption of this primary relationship could lead to a higher incidence of juvenile delinquency, emotional difficulties and anti-social behaviour. To support his hypothesis, he studied 44 adolescent juvenile delinquents in a child guidance clinic.

In a later paper, he reported that 60 children who had spent time apart from their mothers in a tuberculosis sanatorium before the age of 4 showed lower school achievement.

Mary Ainsworth, for instance, published a reassessemnt in a 1962  World Health Organisation paper ‘Deprivation of maternal care: A Reassessment of its Effects’ by Mary Ainsworth et al. [6]  This contains a chapter called ‘Review of Findings and Controversy,’ in which the distinction between ‘separation’ from a parent and ‘parental deprivation’ is carefully set out in an unbundled way.

It was written with Bowlby’s approval as a vehicle to present the more recent research and developments of the 1960s and to address misapprehensions. This publication also attempted to address the previous lack of evidence on the effects of paternal deprivation.

Bowlby has been accused by his peers, e.g. Prof Rutter, of not distinguishing between deprivation and privation – the latter being the complete lack of an attachment bond, rather than its loss. Rutter stresses that the quality of the attachment bond is the most important factor, rather than just deprivation in the critical period. A view shared by American Prof. Warshak (a similar criticism, privation, can be made of Anna Freud work with her 6 jewish refugee sample).

It was not until 1995 that Dr Patricia Morgan pointed out the inconsistencies in many of these early studies. Firstly, they were of small sample sizes which skews any conclusions and secondly but most importantly, only mother absence was examined – not the presence or absence of fathers.

Anna Freud’s series of book regarding “the best interests of the child” came at a time when the judiciary where searching for a scientific foundation for their custody awards. Her findings powerfully reinforced the gathering assumptions of the judiciary in Britain and the USA.

In the instance of Britain the judge appointed to interpret the new law was taken ill and his place was taken by Roger Ormrod who piloted the changes brought by the 1969 Divorce Reform Act. According to another judge (Robin Dunn) he had no time for fathers and single-handedly forced through his interpretations of the radically new law.

But what of the authors of Anna Freud, Joseph Goldstein and Albert J. Solnit ?

  • Anna, as was stated earlier, was dysfunctional in many ways. She was untrained and only qualified to be a school teacher – but her name was the totem required in the psychoanalytical world
  • Joseph Goldstein, although a Yale law professor, he studied and practiced meditation from 1967 onwards under the guidance of eminent teachers from India, Burma, and Tibet.
  • Albert J. Solnit was a Yale psychiatrists and director of its Child StudyCenter. He opened his classes to law students He was the link between social policy and law makers. For instance, one of his students in the late 1960s was Senator Hillary Clinton.
  • Dorothy Burlingham, whose name sometimes appears on some early copies, was also wholly untrained

The Freudian doctrine rather overshadowed what Bowlby had actually said. It also eclipsed the conclusion that Roman & Haddad (1978) were groping towards in their studies.

The attraction of the Freudian doctrine was perhaps its simplistic concept that allowed non-initiates to believe they had attained some form of profound truth.

From the very beginning Freud, Goldstein and Solnit appear to have considered that joint residence posed some kind of threat. They maintained that authority over a child’s life needs to be clearly allocated to one parent, and that children suffer painful loyalty conflicts if they maintain contact with two parents who are not in a harmonious relationship with each other.

Indeed, examination of the contemporaneous photographs of the Hampstead Nurseries and of Bulldogs Banks show only a few male therapists and clinicians working with the children (see the Anna Freud Centre website picture right dated 1941). They are not seen as threats but biological fathers are excluded by a cordon sanitaire, i.e. quarantined.

This theory has now been challenged and shown not to be wholly incorrect but that  it flies in the face of real parents experiences. What married couple have not disciplines or control their children together as a team ? And how often is it that married couples have slightly different ‘take’ on the same situation yet their children grow up (as outcomes prove) to be better adjusted in all departments ?

It is therefore legitimate to ask just how relevant is the work of Freud to children in divorce custody cases. Equally, how relevant is the work of Bolwby and his acolytes

Today the answer can be found in ‘The International Journal of Psychoanalysis’ where a review by H. Sheehan-Dare of “Infants without Families” [7] comes to some unambiguous conclusions:

Q.1. Does the institution child develop differently from the child brought up in a  family ?

A.    Yes

Q.2. Why does a child develop differently in an institution ?

A.    Anna Freud’s own research in 1943 at two residential nurseries (housing  about 90 children) tells us the reasons.

To quote Freud with regard children in institutions:

  • ‘Superficial observation of children of this kind leaves a conflicting picture. They resemble, so far as outward appearances are concerned, children of middle-class families: they are well developed physically, properly nourished, decently dressed, have acquired clean habits and decent table manners, and can adapt themselves to rules and regulations. So far as character development is concerned, they often prove … not far above the standard of destitute or neglect ….” – Anna Freud: Part 3 – The curse is cast, see  https://motoristmatters.wordpress.com/category/anna-freud/   

In other words the superficiality of middle class standards, e.g. table manners, have been learnt by rote but the underlying quality of the personality is absent.

What could not have been deduced by Freud in her work over a short number of years with infants and very young children are the adolescent ‘outcomes’.  If we turn to more up-to-date studies, e.g. News-Medical in Child Health News (25th May 2004), we find that the age old Gold Standard is still valid. It found that “Children’s behaviour is linked to contact with real father” [8]

In uncomplicated English, while there may have been an inkling from August Aichhorn’s pre-war work with young male anti-social behaviour later in life [9] Anna Freud can have been little idea as to the extent and depth and how fatherlessness would also affect girls:

  • “…father-absence continued to affect the rate of early sexual activity and teenage pregnancy exhibited by daughters. In addition rates were highest for early father-absence, in the middle for late father-absence, and lowest for father-present adolescent girls.” [see also “A Daughter’s Invisible Scars”  http://motoristoppression.wordpress.com/2012/05/14/13/]

Finally, the single factor undercutting all of Freud’s work was her concentration on ‘orphaned’ and ‘traumatised’ children. Ever since her work was published “experts” and legal commentators have persisted in amalgamating public law, which deals with ‘orphaned’ and ‘traumatised’ children and private law which deals with divorce and far more balanced children than those from Nazi concentration camps or ‘blitzed’ traumatised children.

The following table is again taken from Dr. Linda Nielsen paper. It shows a goodly number of shared residence surveys and the sample size of each. But it also characterises the smallness of the surveys that have been used to support maternal custody only dogma over the years.

The over-emphasis on maternal bonding and attachment theory is today recognised as misplaced. Yes, it is still valuable and vital but its value and vitality lies in its conjunction with paternal bonding.

Joan B. Kelly and Michael E. Lamb (July 2000) maintain that:

  • “The evening and overnight periods (like extended days with nap times) spent with non-residential parents are especially psychologically important not only for infants but for toddlers and young children as well.” – Family and Conciliation Courts Review, Vol. 38 NO. 3.

Kelly & Lamb go on to say, and I quote:

  • Evening and overnight periods provide opportunities for crucial social interactions and nurturing activities, including bathing, soothing hurts and anxieties, bedtime rituals, comforting in the middle of the night, and the reassurance and security of snuggling in the morning after awakening, that 1-hour or 2-hour of visiting time cannot provide. These everyday activities promote and maintain trust and confidence in the parents while deepening and strengthening child-parentattachments. 

According to Kelly & Lamb this can only lead to the inevitable conclusion that despite historic claims to the contrary:

  •  “. . . there is absolutely no evidence that children’s psychological adjustment or the relationships between children and their parents are harmed when children spend overnight periods with their other parents.”

Kelly & Lamb also take the opportunity to correct an often mis-quoted study by Solomon (1997) which reported “high levels of insecure infant-mother and infant-father attachment” when parents lived apart, although toddlers who spent overnights with both their fathers and mothers were not significantly more likely to have insecure relationships than those children who did not have overnight visits with both parents:

  • Indeed, as articulated above, there is substantial evidence regarding the benefits of these regular experiences.
  • Aside from maintaining and deepening attachments, overnights provide children with a diversity of social, emotional, and cognitively stimulating experiences that promote adaptability and healthy development.
  • In addition, meaningful father-child relationships may encourage fathers to remain involved in their children’s lives by making them feel enfranchised as parents. – Family and Conciliation Courts Review, Vol. 38 NO. 3.

Other advantages of overnights which are often neglected in debate are the normal combination of leisure and “real” time that extended parenting affords each parent. As married couples with small children will identify with, a week end away from the kids recharges their own batteries.

This enhances each parent’s ability to stay abreast of the constant and complex changes in the child’s development, opportunities for effective discipline and teaching that are central to good parenting. It also facilitates an opportunity to ‘reconnect’ with the child in a meaningful way.

In contrast, brief 2-hour visits remind the infant that the visiting parents exist but do not provide the broad array of parenting activities that anchor the relationships in their minds (Kelly & Lamb 2000 p. 306).

And as Tripp & Cockett were to discover in their Exeter University study of 1991 (pub’d HMSO), it is the ‘departure’ and repeated departure of the parent (the father) that is the most enduring to a child’s memory. To break the bond between the child and one parent so arbitrarily is to destroy continuity of care. They argued, in 1991, that joint residence or shared parenting as it is now termed, allows both adults the gratification of parenting. 
The giant figure of David Popenoe, Professor of Sociology at Rutgers University, is an internationally acclaimed expert in these matters and concurs. For over 20 year Popenoe has been at the forefront of social analysis. He has written widely on family structure and children in learned journals and books. His 1996 book “Life Without Father: Compelling New Evidence That Fatherhood and Marriage Are Indispensable for the Good of Children and Society” set new standards.
Popenoe found – and he is one among many others since – that the relationship between ‘family structure’ and ‘crime’ is so strong that it erases the relationship between race and low income and crime. In short a city’s crime rate was set not by social exclusion or ethnicity but by a city’s divorce rate and the resulting fatherlessness rate.

American Barbara Dafoe Whitehead, a lecturer and author, has shown that nationally in the US, more than 70% of all juveniles in state reform institutions come from fatherless homes. [10]

Given that America is in many regards 20 year ahead of Europe in social issues, if not 30 years in some less advanced countries, we are left to draw our own conclusions.

Some of the studies shown in the Table below – as Neilson states – tend to have less random and smaller samples and some by today’s standards do not take into account confounding variables – those of the parents’ income, parents’ education, and levels of conflict. The following Table shows the number of studies into shared parenting, and the outcomes for residential custody type. 

That said, these 20 studies are, by their very diversity and their spread over time, a representative sample of the results that can be expected from larger surveys i.e. longitudinal. Not surprisingly they are uniform in their conclusions about the positive aspects of shared parenting.

Each of these 20 studies addresses at least one of four questions:-

  1. Do most parents in shared parenting families differ in significant ways from other divorced parents ? Specifically, are they far better off financially or far more cooperative than other divorced parents ? Is shared parenting feasible only for a relatively small, selective group of parents.
  2. Are there any advantages for children who grow up in shared parenting families compared to those who live almost exclusively with their mother ?
  3. How do adolescents and young adults who have been raised in shared parenting and maternal residence families feel about the living arrangement that their parents chose for them? Which of these two parenting plans did they feel was in their best interest ?
  4. How does the quality of the father–child relationship compare in shared parenting versus mother residence families ?

The sample sizes listed in the table above are worryingly small from a statistical and/or and actuarial perspective. Yet this is the basis on which headlines are founded and legal changes made.

Out of the surveys listed above only 4 stand out as large enough to be reliable for form proper judgments:

  1. Juby, Burdais & Gratton            (2005) – 121 and 657 families
  2. Kaspiew et al                            (2009) – 750 and 4,250 families
  3. Melli & Brown                            (2008) – 597 and  597 families
  4. Smyth                                       (2010)  – 80 and 2,222 parents

If it appears perverse to lament and moderately criticise the lack of larger studies we have to remind ourselves that surveys attempting to show negative impact on children of shared parenting often have sample sizes of 48 or 131.

We should not be overly concerned regarding size of samples which support how important shared parenting is for children because they serve only to reinforce what is found time and again in longitudinal study data.

Britain is lucky to have several long-term longitudinal surveys:

  • British Birth Cohort Studies first begun in 1946
  • the Millennium Cohort Study – a more recent addition
  • and more recently still, comparative data from a range of European and Nordic countries and the USA. 

Other European and Nordic countries have similar studies and in Sweden a survey of over 1 million children was undertaken (for example, “Centre for Epidemiology, National Board of Health and Welfare, Stockholm, Sweden” report in the ‘Lancet’ Jan 2003,  -findings: Children with single parents showed increased risks of psychiatric disease, suicide or suicide attempt, injury, and addiction. http://www.lancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140-6736(03)12324-0/abstract).

Using the national longitudinal data academics, researchers such as Prof. Kathleen Kiernan and Dr Patricia Morgan, have since the 1990s been able to follow the progress of children from birth into adulthood. They have noted the educational attainment, employment, economic position, and family formation and dissolution patterns of these same children later in their lives. These events are referred to as “outcomes” and should not be confused with ‘results’ which imply an immediacy or reaction.

The analysis of both US and British data showed that daughters living in households where there was no father figure were less likely to attain good school qualifications, to be more economically disadvantaged and more likely to have become young mothers than their contemporaries in the reference group of families where a father was present.

Even ‘after controlling’ for pre-disruption characteristics of the child and the family, including emotional problems, ‘cognitive’ achievement, and socio-economic status striking differences remained. The results show that by age 23, those children whose parents had divorced were more likely to leave home (because of friction), to cohabit, and to have a child outside marriage than were those whose parents did not divorce.

Where are these children, now aged 23 to 30, supposed to have learnt the importance of having a father in their home ? Not at school – and certainly not from government policies or agencies.

How are they to reason out that the predicament they now finds themselves in was to a large degree pre-ordained by divorce and the acts of courts which pushed their father away from them ?

Courts don’t set out to be monsters and mothers certainly don’t want to be seen as monsters – they only want what is best for the child. Only creatures in film like King Kong, Alien or Godzilla are the true monsters. Bowlby and Freud don’t want to be seen as monsters and indeed they are not. They are imperfect human beings complete with human frailties – and thus prone to errors.

After all, courts are only awarding child custody on the allegedly ‘scientific’ basis they have been told is reliable namely the work of Bowlby and Freud. The fact that the science in now 70 years old and is based on the experiences orphans should prompt, among the academic world, some rapid reassessment. But that allegedly scientific basis for assessing the child’s best interests has been more properly described in recent years as flawed by its indeterminancy.

So ‘monster’ might yet be the epitaph for courts when young people scarred by their parents’ divorce and custody process seriously begin looking for answers to their problems in the future.

For this reason alone it would be prudent for policy makers to now make up for lost time and, as the Americans would say, “get ahead of the curve.

Research on children of divorce spanning several decades has revealed at least four conclusions which are relevant to the growing interest in shared residential and ‘parenting plans.’

As Linda Nielsen puts it in her paper:

  • “All four are compatible with the assumption that shared residential parenting might be associated with equal or better outcomes for children than maternal residence—above all, to stronger and more enduring bonds between fathers and children.”

As recently as 2010 a paper by Amato & Dorius found that children of divorce benefit most when their father is actively engaged in their lives across a wide range of daily activities and when he is has an authoritative rather than a permissive or an authoritarian parenting style.

If this is true – and it is a conclusion found in many papers – then limiting fathering time to weekends or to brief weekday visits is less likely to benefit children because the kinds of activities that build strong parent–child bonds and promote authoritative parenting are less likely to occur.

Getting ready for school, working together on homework, going shopping and being together in spontaneous, unstructured ways brings mothers closer to their children, and Linda Nielsen finds the same is true of time and activities shared between children and fathers.

It is this unexceptional, almost mundane, time together engaged in a broad range of daily activities with their father that promotes the best outcomes for children 15 years ago in 1997, a group of 18 nationally renowned experts in psychology, sociology, social welfare, and law concluded that divorced fathers needed more extensive and more “regular” kinds of time with their children to maintain a quality relationship (see Lamb, Sternberg, & Thompson, 1997).

It was the sociologist, social reformer and theologian Amatai Etzione who some years ago realised the truism that ” Quality time is found within quantity time.”  The idea that quality time alone (that gold standard advocated and disseminated for years) can make up for lost amounts of normal fatherly time is here revealed as preposterous.

We discover, when we overview all the available data regarding fathers and their children, that when children live only with their mother, the majority of their fathers end up spending too little of the “right kind” of high-quality, authoritative parenting time with them (Amato, Meyers, & Emery, 2009).

Fatherhood in crisis ?

This alone has profound effects on both children and fathers.These profound effects among children can be depression and self-harm drink and drug abuse etc. Profound effects among fathers also include depression but their self-harm turns into attempting suicide. Various reports allude to men feeling ddepressed, repressed and objectified. Modern fathers are said to be ‘less fertile’ and feel as if they are not essential to parenting. [11] Have we made so many artificial social constructs, barriers and models that we are close to the point of implosion ? 

It is popular to portray “man” in the 21st century as being in something of a muddle but that is to misread the situation. Men are being pulled in all directions and asked to be all things to all men; they are expected to be new man; the hands-on-dad; to show emotion; to have a feminine side; to be the traditional bread winner; be the strong silent type of romantic novels and yet always to be the ‘hero.’

And if this is true not only of men but of women. How many of them feel pulled in all directions, something their mothers never experienced ? How many of them also have fertility problems ? According to 2008 research reported in the Guardian, a mothers chances of miscarriage, despite all the advances in lifestyle and medicine, have increased in recent times (see also 1st Dec 2011) and have actually doubled in some instances (3rd Aug 2008).

Non-Resident Fathers

Historically non-resident fathers have been officially marginalised and it should come as no surprise that the engagement with their children which had been encouraged throughout their married life turns into a disconnection and leaves them feeling depressed and repressed.

Non-Resident Fathers in Britain”, was a sub-set of divorce much overlooked so the exploration by Bradshaw, Stimson, Williams & Skinner from the Uni. of York in 1997 of these fathers was much overdue.

Bradshaw, Stimson et al showed a linkage between the mothers’ vetoing of access rights for fathers and the fall in their compliance to pay child support. When fathers were allowed unfettered visitation by mutual arrangements and these times were not inexplicably changed at short notice, child support payments were more regular and more dependable (and larger). Further, the number and length of times a father visited his child increased as the gate keeping behaviour of the mother decreased.

Week-end Dad

In her 2011 paper Linda Nielsen finds that “only 30% of maternal residence children spend time with their father weekly and another third have not seen himatall in the past year.”

The typical custody arrangement of today only allows fathers and children to be together every other weekend and on occasional weeknights for a part of the evening.

This means that most children only live with their father 14% of the time. These statistics suggest that maternal residence might not be the best parenting arrangements for many children because it might not be the best way to encourage or promote high-quality fathering time.

Academics at the University of Newcastle, carried out a study in 2008 which found that men tended to pay slightly more attention to their sons than their daughters (and in real life one can suppose mothers spend a little more time with their daughters).

The study looked at more than 11,000 British men and women, born in 1958. Children whose fathers spent more time with them appeared had a higher IQ and were more socially mobile than those who had received little attention. [12] (This is the key socialisation role that fathers uniquely play in the lives of all of their children).

The researchers, lead by Dr Daniel Nettle, warned that it was not enough for parents to live together, but that a father should be actively involved in a child’s life to benefit their development, adding:

  • “What was surprising about this research was the real sizeable difference in the progress of children who benefited from paternal interest and how thirty years later, people whose dads were involved are more upwardly mobile.
  • “The data suggest that having a second adult involved during childhood produces benefits in terms of skills and abilities that endure throughout adult life”

The differences, according to Dr. Nettle, were still detectable by the age of 42.

However, this 2008 finding should not surprise us because the Oxford University’s study which began tracked the lives of 17,000 individuals born in 1958 confirms – as do other longitudinal studies from around the world that:

  • “The father’s vital role in giving his child the start to a successful future….”

The study by Flouri & Buchanan was published in March 2002 found that close paternal involvement not only improves academic performance but also relationships and health:

  • “The benefits are greatest for youngsters who establish a strong bond from at least the age of seven.”
  • “The highest scorers performed best at school, socially and in their marital relationships. After inspection of all the factors influencing a child’s later marital success, such as mental health, academic achievement and emotional behaviour, the influence of a father was most telling.” 
  • “Daughters benefiting from a strong paternal bond were less likely to have mental health problems and boys were less likely to get into trouble with the police.”

Similar results have been documented by other long-term investigations. For instance, a 36 year long longitudinal study in the U.S. found that the children of affectionate fathers were much more likely in there forties to be happily married and mentally healthy and to report good relationships with friends (Franz, McClelland, & Weinburger 1991).

Other examples are Warshak 1992 study and Snarey 1993 study which both found that the child with an available father, both in the early and the adolescent years, is more companionable and responsible as an adult.

It is important to note at the outset that this article does not include studies based on the 5% to 10% of parents in the high-conflict group.

High-conflict families tend to be the Trojan horse most often used in debate to counter the argument in favour of shared parenting so a closer examination of the phenomenon is called for.

Other methods of confusing the issue of shared parenting is to lump it together with having shared parenting forced upon couples by the courts or have courts impose a rigid 50/50 spilt in time allocation between both parents. The issue can on occasions be conflated with the matter of Parental Alienation Syndrome (PAS) as if it is some sort of permanent barrier to shared parenting.

We shall now quickly deal with these few topics before returning to the main text.

‘High-conflict’ families

Adults in what are termed ‘high-conflict’ families display unrelenting hostility and/or conflicts towards themsleves and others. They number only a small percentage of couples (married and unmarried) and are characterised by their generally physical aggressiveness and the resorting to threatening behaviour. Every town’s police force will have a handful of these types of parents and they will also be known to Social Services. It follows that designing a ‘parenting plan’ for this type of abnormal parent is a challenging if not an almost impossible task.

Domestic violence tends to be far more concentrated among women where the sub-sets is; unmarried, single, unemployed (or working part time) poorly educated, and living is subsidised state housing. Domestic violence among married and divorced women is a faction of the former rate (source HORS 191). Not surprisingly high-conflict familes are more likely to be found in this sub-set.

The assessment in Britain some decades ago, and based on court cases, was that implacable mothers were a tiny but vocal minority. Such mothers presented a dilemma for courts who saw their power usurped as  the implacable mother brazenly flaunted their Orders. This situation prevented amicably agreed “parenting arrangement.” Time has moved on and fashions change – now the bête noir is the high conflict family. CAFCASS, which in Britain is responsible for helping families go through divorce and for writing welfare reports, estimate the number of ‘intractable’ cases (their term for high conflict families and implacable mothers) as no more then 5% of the cases they see.

Linda Nielsen writes in her 2011 paper that:

  • Because these high-conflict couples consume so much time in family court and because their conflicts often involve physical abuse, much of the discussion of and objections to shared parenting have been focused on them –  (Johnston, Roseby, & Kuehnle, 2009; McIntosh, Burns, Dowd, & Gridley, 2010; Kuehnle & Drozd, 2012).

They unfortunately also provide rich pickings for surveys which in due course receive extensive attention in the professional journals and the mainstream media. A case in point is the use Australian Jennifer McIntosh has made of this sub-set in her ambition to actively sought to disrupt legislation in Australia, Britain and Israel.

NB. these ‘professional journals’ are not available to the public in many cases and so canot be scruntinised by anyone who has not paid the joining fee. The mainstream media is also shut out and has to rely on Press Releases and summaries. Therefore, no in-depth analysis can be done and the Press becomes enslaved to a small coterie.

The personal agenda of Jennifer McIntosh is anyone’s guess but it allows us, at this juncture, to highlight other aparently unrelated issues which are normally used to deter the adoption of shared parenting, e.g. the false claim that it somehow leads to greater domestic violence.

Dysfunctional experts

One suspects that too often its is personality and ego rather than science and rigour that prevails. From the earliest days there has been an unspoken link between bipolar disorders and violence high conflict in the home where even the alleged world experts have skeletons in their cupboards. Look at Dorothy Burlingham, her husband was bipolar. Then look at Anna and Sigmund Freud. Her father  would beat her and she developed sexual fanaticises (which came first is not clear from the literature). In 1919, aged 25, Anna developed clinical jealousy, depression and masochistic tendencies. What confidence can we have in doctrines when some of the author themsleves are from suspect backgrounds ?

In 1923 Anna Freud published a paper which some have seen as ‘autobiographical’, recounting her experiences; ‘My father is beating the child whom I hate.” (Ref. “The Relation of Beating Phantasies to a Day Dream”). Later Anna Freud and Burlingham began a life long lesbian relationship.

Equal Parenting strictly enforced

This is a red herring,  a decoy. There is a plan to conflate and confuse shared parenting to mean Equal Parenting.  As has been stated earlier, this is not the case. A strict Equal Parenting regime is not what is required by men’s and fathers’ groups worldwide, not least for the practical reasons that it would intrude on daily work commitments. Some foolish politicians have interpreted shared parenting as ‘Equal Parenting’ and have made ridiculous statements about cutting children’s time in half. [13]

People like McIntosh and her colleagues (in 2010) are often cited to support the claim that shared parenting is harmful for most children. In a British law school journal Liz Trinder (2010) wrote a “review of recent research on shared parenting” which was actually based on only two studies of shared parenting families – one of which is McIntosh’s own study. The political leanings of both Liz Trinder and McIntosh are clearly colouring their objectivity (similarly with Mavis Maclean’s and Joan Hunt’s work on this area). Both Trinder and McIntosh do not have the confidence to cite contrary opions which is common (indeed, expected), in most reputable papers.

Linda Nielsen discovered (as I had also found), that the problem with McIntosh’s studies (and various others of a similar persuasion) was that they are based on small, non-random samples – in McIntosh’s study, only 69 of shared parenting families in one casewere used, most of whom were in the high-conflict groups and only 16 of whom had voluntarily chosen shared parenting. In another McIntosh study the sample was still too small at around 131.

But closer examination of the actual paper and not the ‘summary’ or the ‘press release’ showed that the researchers had to conclude that the young children’s higher anxiety scores were more closely correlated with the mother’s poor parenting skills, the father’s low education (and resulting parental’ high conflict),  than with the shared residential parenting or high conflict alone (McIntosh et al., 2010).

This does not, of course, make for attention grabbing headlines.

The work of supposed researchers that include Liz Kelly, Joan Hunt, Liz Trinder, Brenda Neale and Jenn McIntosh all manage to insert in the text, either as a fact or simply as an act of faith, that such shared parenting when forced upon couples proves unworkable.

Who is arguing with that ? Father’s organisations do not want custody arrangements forced upon anyone. Father’s know all to well that forced contact arrangements do not work.

At present we have parenting arrangements forced upon parents and we know how useless they are – indeed, that is why a change to mutually agreed shared parenting is being sought.

Parental Alienation Syndrome (PAS)

New psychiatric disorders among children were first noted in the 1970s as the number of divorce rose followed by child-custody disputes and the intensity increased. Internationally, child contact (custody arrangement), after parental separation or divorce has become a highly contentious issue as countries unconsciously drift towards harmonising their divorce and custody regimes. Some parents do not want to be divorced against their will and many do not want to lose contact with their children.

The pioneer in the field of ‘parental alienation’ was Richard Gardener. According to Gardner, PAS is characterised by a cluster of 8 symptoms that appear in the child (see Appendix). Gardner and others have divided the 8 PAS symptoms into; mild; moderate; and severe levels.The alienation of the child’s affections begins with a campaign of denigration and hatred against the targeted parent. Frivolous allegations may be made about the targeted parent (very often the father) or absurd reasons given for hating, criticising, or disapproving of the other parent.

The public’s assumption is that alienation of a child affection must resemble a mushroom thatinexplicably springs up overnight. But it doesn’t just happen like that- it is the result of coaching and a child’s fear of being unloved and the wish to feel the love ofatleast one parent.

And contrary to how courts and ‘professionals’ behave, once PAS is claimed or established, PAS is not permanent. According to Dr Kirk Weir, who, as a court expert witness has given evidence in hundreds of cases, even the toughest resistance toward the alienated parent can be dissolved into a normal loving contact, sometimes within minutes, sometimes after an hour or so.  [14]

In this singular instance ‘enforcement’ by the court has wholly beneficial results:

  • “ . . . This enforced contact was successful in all cases involving children under 5, in 80% of cases with children aged 5-7, and in 40% of older children.”

Notice how the greater the age (and presumably the longer the delay before professional ‘intervention’) the less will be the success rate. His conclusion is that:

  • ” . . .  alienation is a powerful but unstable defence mechanism found in children who are torn between loyalty to the main carer and feelings for the other parent in high conflict contact cases.”

CAFCASS & Whitehall

To round off this diversion I would like go back to CAFCASS which is responsible for writing reports for the divorce court on the welfare and allocation of children caught up in their parent’s divorce. In the process we shall revisit some of the distortions and economies of truth that frequently plague the issue of divorce and custody.

In 2002 meeting held with senior civil servants led to them insisting that the Children Act 1989 was never intended to be used for shared parenting. Ten years later a document which can now be found on the internet shows that shared parenting was the intention of the Act but that unofficially Government advised judges thatthey should never use it. This frustrated any reform CAFCASS could have brought to child allocation. 

Secondly, and again linked to CAFCASS the minister responsible, Lord Falconer, asked the statistical service ONS to provide him with data showing that only a small percentage of parents were unhappy with custody arrangements. The figure he received in 2004 was that only 10% of parents were ‘unhappy with custody decisions and therefore the problem was not big enough to warrant time being spent on it. The figures he relied upon were taken from the ONS’s Omnibus Survey – a survey of only of 935 adults. Once again, smallness of sample size lead to inaccuracies.

The truth of the matter is that each year, to quote a government official report, “between 150,000 and 200,000 parental couples separate”. Ten percent of a divorce/separation of thattotal would give rise to 15,000 ‘unhappy parents’ Yet CAFCASS deals with between with between 45,000 and 65,000 private law cases of unhappy parents every year (three of four times the 10% figure).

In CAFCASS’s 2004 Annual Report 33,803 private law cases involving 73,937 children are ‘handled by CAFCASS. In 2011 there were 45,000 separate private law applications [15] but those private law applications involved – and this is the key number –  147,000 children.[16]

Worryingly for the public is that parents in the lower working class are not alone in attracting the attention of government child officials. The educated middle class parents and mothers who normally would have had no involvement with Social Services are finding themselves increasingly tangled up as officialdom expands it regime of safeguard ‘screening’ procedures.[17]

Also worryingly for the public is that Social Services professionals cannot be relied upon according to a spokesman for Britain’s CAFCASS child welfare service:

  •  “Social services don’t always understand our role properly either.”

Our presentation today advocating the greater merits of shared parenting over any other alternative concluded by stating the obvious – we need to legislate and change policy not for the 5% or 10% but for the 95%.

We are not advocating change for change sake nor are we advocating change to benefit one parent over the other. No, we are advocating change for those 147,000 children that hardly get a mention and hare hidden inside the less alarming 45,000 figure.

We cannot allow an unrepresentative 5% to hold society hostage against much needed reform.

If 90% of women wanted access to birth control or, say, 90% of women wanted to be screened to detect the early onset of breast cancer (or cervical cancer), would we let a 5% or 10% minority who were vehemently opposed to the measures on ethical grounds win the argument ? No.

Our presentation today, therefore, has concentrated on the silent majority of 90% of divorced parents and excluded the small unrepresentative 10% of couples that fall into the conflict / dysfunctional category. They are at present – and should continue to be dealt with – under a separate and quite tailored scheme suitable for their needs and situation.

Overnight ‘sleep-overs’

It has been a feature of McIntosh’s argument that while it may be satisfactory for, say, 7 year olds to spend time at their fathers house it is totally unsuitable for children under the age of 2. According to the Australian Association for Infant Mental Health’s ”guidelines for protecting the very young child’s sense of comfort and security’‘:

  • ”Prior to the age of two years, overnight time away from the primary care-giver should be avoided, unless necessary”

Belinda Fehlberg shares McIntosh’s views and since 2006 has written profusely in her support. Fehlberg is well placed to get the McIntosh narrative and agenda into the mainstream press as she is on the editorial board of the Australian Journal of Family Law.

Consequently, “Shared custody a mistake for the under-2s, say guidelines” was the headlines in the Sydney Moring Herald of Dec 2011. [18]  Reading the details, however, it is clearly a press release aimedat maximising self- publicity. Based on a study of only 258 children from separated parents the gist of it said that non-custodial parents, i.e. 90% of whom are separated fathers, should see children up to three times a week and then gradually phase in overnight visits after the second birthday.

This is probably what happens in most instances, so nothing radical there. But then the objectivity is ruptured by muddling it with high conflict family and family courts.

Reputable academics, such as Edward Kruk, who are leaders in their field see nothing wrong with sleep-overs at Dad’s house:

  • “The most recent research strongly supports a shift away from the “one size fits all,” “winner take all” sole custody framework toward the notion of shared parental responsibility.”
  • “Shared parental responsibility is in keeping with current caregiving patterns, as the majority of mothers and fathers are now sharing responsibility for child care in twoparent families.”

14% Dads

We touched on the phenonenon of the 14% Dad in an earlier section Five nights per fortnight can also be written as 10 days per month or 120 days per year. This amounts to a little over 32% of the child’s time spent with one or other parent.

However, at present the normal ‘contact’ time awarded to fathers in the majority of cases is alternating weekend visiting, i.e. once a fortnight, and possibly a mid-week visit.

On the barest basis of solely alternating weekend visiting, a father would see his child for 14% of the year (2 x daytime visits, limited to less than 12 hours and no overnight ‘sleep-over’ [51/365 =  14%]).

Sometimes a patient father can gradually work towards being given an overnight ‘sleepover’ concession. This would raise his time allocation to 19% (3 days per fortnight [72/365 = 19%])

Were a father be allowed to see his child for 3 days every week (and not fortnightly) the time spent would increase still further to 39% (144 days = 39%).

However, all these figures can be a misleading since the child has to be returned as night and so it could be argued the time a father spends with his child is only half a day since the child returns at night to the mother’s residence. For this reason the regime is called sole mother custody for the mother has the child for 365 days of the year and there is no sharing.

School holiday periods can provide additional time to be spent with fathers; however, since this is also prime opportunity to interrupt contact it is not included in the above calculations. Shared parenting would see mothers lose the 365 day monopoly and the child would be shared, perhaps 100 days and nights for the fathers and 200 for the mother.”

At present the average appears to be what some one once called “the 14% Dad” and while this might suit some fathers it acts as a straight jacket. So a “32% Dad” is a vast improvement but one, we suspect, is a rare bird.

There is also confusion in McIntosh’s work in that she ascribes shared parenting to mean a child who stays weekends with his father. This we have earlier referred to as disingenuous by researchers and as the 14% father factor.

Michael E. Lamb who shares with David Popenoe nearly 30 years of independent research experience comes to a very opposite conclusion to McIntosh who was still a post-graduate student in the mid 1990s. In Family and Conciliation Courts Review, Joan B. Kelly & Michael E. Lamb write of evenings and overnight periods spent with the child’s father:

  • “There is absolutely no evidence that children’s psychological adjustment or the relationships between children and their parents are harmed when children spend overnight periods with their other parents.” – Vol. 38 NO. 3, July 2000 297-311  
  • “Evening and overnight periods provide opportunities for crucial social interactions and nurturing activities, including bathing, soothing hurts and anxieties, bedtime rituals, comforting in the middle of the night, and the reassurance and security of snuggling in the morning after awakening, that 1to 2-hour visits cannot provide. These everyday activities promote and maintain trust and confidence in the parents while deepening and strengthening child-parent attachments.”

 Aside from maintaining and deepening attachments Kelly & Lamb go on to state that in the same document:

  • “ . . . . overnight [stays] provide children with a diversity of social, emotional, and cognitively stimulating experiences that promote adaptability and healthy development. In addition, meaningful father-child relationships may encourage fathers to remain involved in their children’s lives by making them feel enfranchised as parents.”

What Critics Say: [ to here ]

How often have we heard this criticism levelled at some fathers; that they “disengage”; that they fail to remain involved in their children’s lives; they are no where to be seen ?

The reason in many cases is that they are pushed away by the custody regime adopted by the country in which they live. They are penalised and made vulnerable by the withholding of visiting time should the parent-with-custody so choose.

Let us re-confirm again – we are speaking here of two separate and distinct groups. One where controversy is not an issue for 90% of divorcing families. While the second encompasses the 5% to 10% of dysfunctional families who present ‘problems’ for professional social workers.

The 90% of divorcing families deserve and better deal than they are getting at the moment – and more to the point the children of those 90% of divorcing families need and deserve and better deal.

It is now generally accepted as an incontrovertible truth – except among die-hard radical feminists – that non-residential fathering time is beneficial and is closely related to the quality and the endurance of the father-and-child relationship.

We would, therefore, expect that fathers and children in shared parenting families would have better relationships than children and fathers who do not live togetherat least a third of the time.

But do they ?

Is there any evidence that shows living together for more than just a couple of weekends a month is associated with stronger or more enduring bonds between fathers and children ?

The answer is very strongly “Yes.”

Even adolescents, who can be the most trying, i.e. most difficult, age group for parent s to handle, who spent only slightly more time with their father had measureable closer relationship compared with those that saw their father only during the day.

This is a generally neglected area but there are interesting snippets dating back as far as the 1990s that gives us a rare glimpse if not insight. From the UK comes this Table depicting how many infants to adolescents are looked after by each sex of divorced parent, where SMH = single mother household and SFH = single father household.

In the Appendix is a more detailed break-down of children by family types and residences of all single families.

Stronger or more enduring bonding between fathers and children held true in a study by Cashmore, Parkinson, & Taylor (2008), even when the overall amount of time both groups spent together with their father was equal and regardless of the amount of ‘conflict’ between the parents.  The supposition by Cashmore, Parkinson, & Taylor, was that this relaxed normative time together helped adolescents and appreciate their bonds with their fathers in more powerful ways.

You might inwardly ask, “Are the number of days spent living with their father directly related to the quality of their relationship years after the parents’ divorce ?”  The answer has to be that quality timed is not a God given right and nor is it guaranteed. Therefore, ‘quantity of time’ must come first, and as with all relationships some of that time will be superb and memorable and other times less so.

Linda Neilson gives us this example as an answer:

  • “ . . . . the most methodologically sophisticated study is based on 1,030 university students whose parents divorced before they were 16 (Fabricius, Diaz, & Braver, 2011).
  • Nearly 400 of them had lived in shared parenting families after their parents separated.
  • The number of days they lived with their fathers each month and the present quality of their relationships were highly correlated. That is to say, the more days they had lived together each month, the higher they rated the quality of their present relationship.
  • The amount of time these fathers and children had spent together after the parents’ divorce ranged from never up to 50% of the time.

The researchers also addressed this complicated question:

  • Does additional time together have any impact on the very worst relationships or only the very best relationships that children had with their fathers ?

To answer this question, the researchers analysed the correlations for the “best” and the “worst” 20% of child-father relationships.

If we assume that the fathers in the top 20% would have had great relationships with their children (regardless of how much time they lived together), then time should not be highly correlated with the relationship quality for this group.

Equally, ‘time’ should not be associated with the quality of the relationship for the 20% with the worst relationships.

In fact, time turned out to be the single pivotal factor.

  • For both the best and the worst relationships, living together for more time was associated with higher quality relationships. In other words, those who lived together more of the time, had the better relationships – especially those who had lived together 30% to 50% of the time.

In other words, both the best and the worst relationships would have been at a much lower levels had not fathers not had the greater time (30% to 50%) allowed to them.

Five other recent studies confirm these results.

  • A survey of 400 university students revealed that of the 80 students who had lived in dual residence families almost all, 93%, said this had been the best parenting plan for them, compared to only 30% of the other students.
  • Nearly 70% of the sole residence students felt it would have been in their best interests to have lived more with their father.
  • More than half (55%) said their fathers had wanted equal residential custody, but their mothers had opposed it. Even those who spent two weekends every month with their fathers said this was not nearly enough time together.
  • The dual residence children had closer relationships with their fathers and their mothers than the others (Fabricius, 2003).
  • Similarly, 3 years after their parents’ divorce, 80% of the children in the 597 shared parenting Wisconsin’s family survey were spending just as much time with their father and were more satisfied with their relationship with him.
  • In contrast, more than half of the children in sole residence families were spending far less time with their fathers and were unhappy about this loss. A number of their relationships had ended altogether (Melli & Brown, 2008).

This brings us to what may prove to be a throny question in the years ahead. A number of countries are pushing ahead with amending a father’s contact/care time. Yet will this be limited to daytime hours ‘quality time’ or will it include residence as the Belgians have chosen ? Will shared parenting also prove to be shared residence (the two may be viewed as quite distinct by some) ?

Linda Nielsen then cites a study from the Netherlands, involving 135 children in shared parenting families. This study, by Spruijt & Duindam (2010), found that children in shared parenting relationship had as close a relationship with both parents as the 2,000 children from intact families.

The father-child relationships were closer than those when the father regularly spent time with the children, where the children lived with their mother (as in mother-custody scenarios).

The Netherlands study is of interest because some radical feminist blog sites, e.g. the Liz Library, use older material claiming research points in the opposite direction:

  • “A recent survey of 9,816 secondary school students in the Netherlands indicates that the level of well being of children living in single mother families is higher than that of students living in two parent families with much parental conflict, the well being of children living in single mother families with no parental conflict and with a great deal of contact with the departed father is lower than that of children living in two parent families without parental conflict and finally, the degree of parental conflict after divorce is more important for the well being of the children than the degree of contact with the departed father (Jaap Dronkers, 1996).” [19]

While a survey of over 9,800 is large enough to be an accurate guide, phrases such as ‘well being’ and ‘parental conflict’ are vague enough to be stretched to one’s own purposes. One also has to question the claim of never having ‘conflict’ in single mother households. Is there ever a time when ‘children living in single mother’ households do not fight or have conflict with one another ?

Why does Dronkers appear to draw comparisons between intact two parent families where there is conflict with single mother households where there is none ? this is fanciful nonsense. all households (intact or SMH) will experince some level of disagreements from time to time

However, a fuller reading of Dronkers’ 1996 paper reveals a different picture. Although written almost 20 years ago his overall findings confirm much of the shortcomings of divorce and single parent up-bringing and endorse that laid out in this article. This exemplifies the bias by way of ‘selective’ editing and quoting that is rampant in this field of endeavour.

A moment ago mention was made of the Wisconsin 2008 family survey involving 597 shared parenting families and an equal number of maternal residence families. Coming more than 10 years after Dronkers’ 1996 paper, the shift in attitudes is palpable.

It has often been asserted – but no proof offered – that shared parenting needs a great deal of co-operation if it is to work and that that it is suitable only for educated and wealthier families, i.e. some form of elite (this false supposition first appeared in a Law Commission report (Britain) in the late 1980s).

However, the Wisconsinstudy – undertaken by Melli & Brown, in 2008 – found that the shared parenting couples were not that much more wealthy, not that more educated, nor were they that more co-operative than the other divorced parents in the study.

Just as an aside, the incomes levels shown in the adjacent Table partially reflect the finding of Gilder, Hewitt and Amneus some 30 years earlier with regard gender and marital status (see ‘wealth creation’ above).

In both groups shown here the mothers’ incomes and educational levels were virtually the same, at$23,000 versus $22,000 for shared versus maternal residence respectively. The percentage of college graduates, at 25%, among both sets of women gives no intrinsic advantage – but which in the past many have claimed. However, the variable for fathers is in their income generation and level of education and reinforces trends seen in other large scale studies.

In regard to money, it is important to note that in the Wisconsin study there has been an increase in shared residential custody for lower income parents.

This is highly significant in cultural terms and social expectations. It would suggest – as Cook & Brown (2006), have pointed out – that this arrangement (i.e. shared residence), is becoming more widespread.

Do not run away with the idea that shared parenting is the perfect answer, where everybody comes out smiling and it has no drawbacks.

It is not a perfect answer to custody allocation problems but it is more perfect than the present regime we have.

For example, the above study (Wisconsin) while underscoring older studies in that fathers in shared residence arrangements had more flexible, child-friendly work hours, however, the quality of the co-parenting relationship was not especially friendly. Some critics of shared parenting have always asserted that it work only where ‘both parents co-operate fully’ – well, apparently this is not true.

The Wisconsin study found both groups (shared residence & maternal residence), were equally likely, in 15% of cases, to describe their relationship as “hostile.” Furthermore, most parents had a distant, business-like relationship that was not conflict free.

Melli & Brown in their study of 2008 say this:

  • “In fact, the shared parenting couples were more likely to have conflicts over childrearing issues (50%) than families where the children lived with their mother (30%), largely because these fathers were so much more engaged in the parenting.”

So the apparent downside is more conflict, sometimes, but it has to be remembered that the conflict is over the quality or style of child rearing not the absence of it by one parent. ‘Society’ wants fathers to be much more hands-on, much more “engaged” in parenting, so in the scheme of things this is a small disadvantage given the huge benefits accruing to the child of having two actively engaged parents.

Benefits accruing from shared parenting are not restricted to children or to fathers but extend to benefit mothers and women with careers. Both parents, mother and fathers, feel less overwhelmed by whatever life throws at them when shared parenting is adopted. Mothers in sole custody households reported being overwhelmed in 30% of instances compared with only 13% of mothers with shared parenting residence arrangements. (Luepnitz, 1991).

Another source of criticism levels at shared parenting which needs to be laid to rest is the notion that children who live at two home addresses feel more unstable or more insecure than those who live with only one of their parents. The assertion is that these children live in a perpetual state of transit, carelessly being “shuffled” from one home to another.

Is it destabilizing; is there any stress and if true, how stressful is it ?

Does it affect girls more than boys or vice versa ?

Does it affect teenage girls if they have two of more boyfriends at one time ?

No study can be found that directly compares levels of ‘insecurity’ or ‘instability’ by comparing children in both kinds of families.

But of the research reviewed – and some of it is incorporated in this text – living in two homes does not appear to create a sense of insecurity, instability, or confusion for children (rather one suspects it can be a relief from the humdrum of the altenative).

If there is an irritation-like element it takes the form of the inconvenience of moving home. Though not intended to compare types of family arrangements two studies of British children aged between 6 and 22 are helpful.

Of the 117 children, 38 lived for an equal time with each parent and 35 lived every weekend with one parent and weekdays with the other. The interviews with the 73 children who were living in both parents’ homes revealed a diverse array of reactions.

Most mentioned only the practical inconveniences such as having to pack-up clothes, toys, and school supplies; letting friends know where they were; keeping up with their things; and needing to be well organised.

As one might expect, adolescents who can be difficult for parents to parent and control tended to find the arrangement of two homes more inconvenient than younger children because it interfered more with their social lives. Most also said it took time to adjust to each parent’s household rules and make the emotional shift from one home to the other.

Despite these inconveniences and the need to make adaptations, most preferred living in two homes to living with only one parent.

However, we have to accept that a small number of children will find it ‘disorientating.’  But even among this group the appreciation of shared time with mother and father was recognised and valued. Akin one suspects of spending part of the weekend at ‘grandads. Some children were found to dislike moving back because they found one of the parents “more boring” than the other.

At the opposite end of the spectrum were these that thrived on the routine of having two homes. Among these children it was found thatthey were the most relaxed and socialised of all children.

Some critics of shared parenting do so because they claim it exacerbates litigation. They also claim that it adds fuel to the divorce industry professionals such as custody evaluators, mental health professionals resulting in cases being drawn out for months and each case costs the tax payer more than it should.

But this is to misrepresent the truth. Sole custody to one or other parent will invariably induce a propensity for a legal challenge in order to regain some sense of equality. Shared parenting in whatever shape or form, however, depends on cooperation and agreement before reaching court, usually by mediation.

In that single correction of an untruth the cost-saving to the tax payer is revealed and the nonsense of supposedly exacerbating litigation shown to be exactly that, a nonsense.

END

Appendix A

 ‘Parental Qualities of the Father

by David Popenoe

The overwhelming weight of research shows that two-parent families are best for children, and that there are serious problems for children in single-parent families. There is a poverty of research into the differences in outcomes for children living with single mothers or single fathers, but the small available sources suggest that men are likely to be equally good parents. 

Differences as parents between men and women are probably exaggerated. Only a few decades ago, parental mortality produced about as many single-parent families as are now produced by divorce (Popenoe, p. 78).

Parental mortality only recently ceased to be the normal mode of parent loss, and only within a minority of the world’s cultures. Thus, from our deepest ancestral roots, fathers have evolved in a world that required them to be capable parents. 

In western European cultures, psychological and social theories of the past several decades have emphasized the mother, and failed to examine the father in child development, but research of roughly the past decade has revealed a surprising complexity and importance in the father’s approach to parenting:

  • “Beyond being merely a second adult or third party, fathers – men – bring an array of unique and irreplaceable qualities thatwomen do not ordinarily bring.” (‘War Over the Family’, by David Popenoe, pub 2008).

These include protector, and male role model. Daughters learn from their fathers how to relate to men and learn that they are worthy of love. 

“…in almost all of their interactions with children, fathers do things a little differently from mothers. What fathers do is not only complementary to what mothers do but … is by all indications important in it’s own right for children rearing.” (‘War Over the Family’, D. Popenoe p.122).

“… an often-overlooked dimension of fathering is play. …the fathers’ style of play seems to have unusual significance. It is likely to be both physically stimulating and exciting. With older children it involves more physical games and teamwork that require the competitive testing of physical and mental skills. It frequently resembles an apprenticeship or teaching relationship: Come on, let me show you how.  (‘War Over the Family’, D. Popenoe p.122).

“…mothers’ play tends to take place more at the child’s level. Mothers provide the child with the opportunity to direct the play, to be in charge, to proceed at the child’s own pace. Kids, at least in the early years, seem to prefer to play with Daddy.

In one study of 2½ year-olds who were given a choice, more than two-thirds chose to play with their fathers.”  (‘War Over the Family’, D. Popenoe pp.122 – 123).

“A committee assembled by the Board on Children and Families of the National Research Council concluded that children ‘learn critical lessons about how to recognize and deal with highly charged emotions in the context of playing with their fathers. Fathers, in effect, give children practice in regulating their own emotions and recognizing others’ emotional clues.”

“A study of convicted murderers in Texas. . . . . [found that] 90% of them either didn’t play as children or played abnormally.”  (‘War Over the Family’, D. Popenoe p.123).

“It’s sometimes said that fathers express more concern for the child’s long-term development, while mothers focus on the child’s immediate well-being. It is clear that children have dual needs that must be met. …. One without the other is a denuded and impaired humanity, an incomplete realization of human potential.” 

“It is ironic that in our public discussion of fathering, it’s seldom acknowledged that fathers have a distinctive role to play. Indeed, it’s far more often said thatfathers should be more like mothers (and thatmen generally should be more like women – less aggressive, less competitive).

While such things may be said with the best of intentions, the effects are perverse. After all, if fathering is no different from mothering, males can easily be replaced in the home by women. It might even seem better. Already viewed as a burden and obstacle to [female] self-fulfilment, fatherhood thus comes to seem superfluous and unnecessary as well.” (‘War Over the Family’, D. Popenoe p.124).

These indispensable attributes of fathering are persistently disregarded in feminist gender analysis of parenting and all feminist literature.

Disregarding the many nuances of parenting cannot avoid negatively impacting on custody and the well being and life chances of children everywhere.

Appendix B

 8 symptoms that appear in the child

 http://www.alienatedparent.org.uk/checklist/

According to Gardner, PAS is characterised by a cluster of eight symptoms that appear in the child. These include a campaign of denigration and hatred by one parent, through the child, against the targeted parent.

  1. A campaign of denigration
  2. Weak, frivolous and absurd rationalisations (excusing the denigration).
  3. Lack of ambivalence.
  4. The “independent thinker phenomenon.”
  5. Reflexive support of the alienating parent in the parental conflict.
  6. Actions of guilt over cruelty to and/or exploitation of the alienated parent.
  7. The presence of borrowed scenarios.
  8. The spread of animosity to the extended family and alienated parent.

Gardner and others divided PAS into mild, moderate and severe levels.

Brazil is so far the only country whose laws recognize and condemns the SAP (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Parental_alienation_syndrome).

Richard A. Gardner M.D. – Clinical Professor of Child Psychiatry at Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeon.

Appendix C

Single parent family types

The Table below lists the wide variety of single parent family types to be found in any society. Those highlighted in Red list the most obvious candidates; the lone father; the widowed mother etc. In the age range 0 – 4 yo, lone fathers look after very few children (though it has to be pointed out that the sample size at 272 is unreliably small, and the accompanying ONS text appears to suggest that most Lone Father are in fact widowers).

Notwithstanding that, note how in this larger array the number of 16-18 yo and over 19+ congregate most among Lone Fathers and Widowed Lone Mothers (13% and 11% respectively).

By contrast, single lone mothers excel at looking after under 4 year olds but those children have left home by the time they are 16, i.e. 63% falling to 0.6%, and at age 19 are totally absent from SMHs.

Divorced mothers, where the father is arguably present on occasions, fare much better at 9% for the 16-18 yo (and 4% for over 19 yo), with separated lone mothers at 4%.

Those numbers in Blue show the whole spectrum of family types including married couples to aid in comparison.

 

Annex

 Future source / reference material

  1. Kathleen Kiernan “The Legacy of Parental Divorce” CASE, 1997.
  2. Patricia Morgan, “Farewell to the Family?” IEA, 1996
  3. Patricia Morgan “The Fragmenting Family: Does it Matter”. IEA
  4. Dennis N. “Families without Fatherhood”, IEA.
  5. Dennis N. “The Invention of Permanent Poverty”, IEA
  6. Bradshaw, J. and Millar, J. (1991) Lone Parent Families in the UK, Department of Social Security Research Report No 6, London: HMSO.
  7. Charles Murray, “Charles Murray and the Underclass – the developing debate” IEA
  8. Seltzer J, “Father by law: effects of joint legal custody on non-resident fathers with children” Demography, Vol. 35, No 2 May 1998.
  9. Seltzer J, “Consequences of marital dissolution for children” 1994
  10. Seltzer J, “The father’s role after separation”.
  11. Seltzer, J. A. (1991) ‘Relationships between fathers and children who live apart: the father’s role after separation’, Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 79-101.
  12. Seltzer, J.A.,Schaeffer,N. C. and Charng, HAW. (1989) `Family ties after divorce. The relationship between visiting and paying child support’, Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 663-677.
  13. Sutton, T (1996) `A socio-economic approach to Child Support Compliance’, Paper presented to the Australian National Family Research Conference, Brisbane 1996.
  14. Amneus D, “The Garbage Generation”.
  15. Farrell W. “The Myth of Male Power”
  16. Farrell W. “Women Can’t Hear What Men Don’t Say”
  17. Clarke, L. (1996) `Father figures – research data comes on stream’, Family Policy Bulletin, November.
  18. Marsh, A. (1993) ‘Absent parenthood’ (unpublished paper).
  19. Pilling, “Escape from Disadvantage”,London. Falmer Press. 1990
  20. “Mean Overall Behavioural Problems scoreper child aged 5-17 by family type and selected demographic and social characteristics:United States” 1988.
  21. Crellin, Pringle & West, “Born illegitimate”. Dec 1988
  22. Dubow & Lester, “Adjustment of Children Born to Teenage Mothers: the Contribution of Risk and Protective Factors “, Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 52.
  23. Lummis, “The Historical Dimensions to Fatherhood: a Case Study 1890-1914, in, The Father Figure”, Tavistock 1982.
  24. Wright, “Sexual Unions and Infant Mortality in Jamaica”. Unpublished manuscript. University of Glasgow, 1994.
  25. Manderbacka, “Marital status as a Predictor of Perinatal Outcomes inFinland”, 1992.
  26. Daly & Wilson, “Body and Mind: Darwinism and the Human Sciences”. LSE. 1993
  27. Dawson, “Family Structure and Children’s Health: United States” 1988, Series 10:178.
  28. Pilling, “Escape from Disadvantage”,London. Falmer Press. 1990
  29. Judge & Benzeval, “Health Inequalities: New Concerns About Children of Single Mothers”, BMA 1993
  30. Lamb, “The Role of Fathers in Development”, 1981.
  31. Cox, “Marriage Adult Adjustment and Early Parenting”, Child Development. Vol. 60. 1989.
  32. Greenstein, B, “The Fragile Male”, Papers and unpublished studies, 1992.
  33. West. “Delinquency: It’s Roots, Careers and Prospects”. 1982
  34. Capaldi & Patterson, “Relations in Parental Transitions to Boy’s Adjustment problems in a Linear Hypothesis, ii) Mothers at Risk for Transitions and Unskilled Parenting”, Developmental Psychology, Vol. 27, 1991, pp 489.
  35. Ferri, “Step Children”, NFER 1984.
  36. Popone D. “Divorce and the Kids: the Evidence”. 1994.
  37. Sampson R., “Urban Black Violence: The Effect of Male Joblessness and Family Disruption”, 1987.
  38. Kissman, Kris “Non-custodial Fatherhood: Research Trends and Issues”, Journal of Divorce & Remarriage, v.28, n.1/2, pp.77: Publication Date: 12-31-97.
  39. Riley & Shaw, “Parental Supervision and Juvenile Delinquency”. Home Office No 83, 1985.
  40. Dominian J, “Marital Breakdown and the Health of the Nation”, One Plus One, 1991.
  41. McLanahan & Booth, “Mother-only Families: Problems, Prospects and Politics”. 1989
  42. Stack. S. “The Impact of Divorce on Suicide in Norway”. 1951-1980. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1989. The Effect of Suicide in Denmark 1961-1980. The Sociological Quarterly. 1986.
  43. Lords and Commons ‘Family and Child Protection Group’, “Family Matters” 1998.
  44. Lords and Commons ‘Family and Child Protection Group’. “The Cost of Family Breakdown”  ISBN 0 9533429 1 3

[*] denote contrary, i.e. pro-sole mother custody views.

  1. * Hewitt P & Leach P, “Social Justice, Children and Families”. 1993
  2. * Coote, Harman & Hewitt, “The Family Way”. IPPR 1990.
  3. * McLanahan, “The Consequences of Single Parenthood for Subsequent Generations”. 1988.

Annex

Quote:  Eighty-five percent of youth in prison have an absent father, 71% of high school dropouts are fatherless, 90% of homeless and runaway children have an absent father, and fatherless children and youth exhibit higher levels of depression and suicide, delinquency, promiscuity and teen pregnancy, behavioral problems and illicit and licit substance abuse, diminished self-concepts, and are more likely to be victims of exploitation and abuse (Stein, Milburn, Zane, & Rotheram-Borus, 2009; Rosenberg and Wilcox, 2006; Crowder and Teachman, 2004; Ellis et al., 2003; Ringback Weitoft, Hjern, Haglund, & Rosen, 2003; Jeynes, 2001; McCue Horwitz et al., 2003; McMunn, Nazroo, Marmot, Boreham, & Goodman, 2001; Blankenhorn, 1995).

Source – Edward Kruk (2012): “Arguments for an Equal Parental Responsibility Presumption in Contested Child Custody.” The American Journal of Family Therapy, 40:1, 33-55

Additional references:

Blankenhorn, D. (1995). Fatherless America: Confronting our most urgent social problem.New York, NY: Basic Books.

Crowder, K., & Teachman, J. (2004). Do residential conditions explain the relationship between living arrangements and adolescent behavior? Journal of Marriage and Family, 66, 721–738.

Ellis, B. J., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Pettit, G. S., & Woodward, L. (2003). Does father absence place daughters at special risk for early sexual activity and teenage pregnancy? Child Development, 74, 801–821.

Jeynes, W. H. (2001). The effects of recent parental divorce on their children’s consumption of marijuana and cocaine. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 35(3/4), 43–64.

McCue Horwitz, S. M., Irwin, J. R., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Bosson Heenan, J. M., Mendoza, J. M., & Carter, A. S. (2003). Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 932–940.

McMunn, A. M., Nazroo, J. Y., Marmot, M. G., Boreham, R., & Goodman, R. (2001).Children’s emotional and behavioural well-being and the family environment: Findings from the health survey for England. Social Science and Medicine, 53,423–440.

Ringback Weitoft, G., Hjern, A., Haglund, B., & Rosen, M. (2003). Mortality, severe morbidity, & injury in children living with single parents in Sweden: A population–based study. The Lancet, 361, 289–295

Rosenberg, J., & Wilcox, W. B. (2006). The importance of fathers in the healthy development of children. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Stein, J. A., Milburn, N. G., Zane, J. I., & Rotheram-Borus, M. (2009). Paternal and maternal influences on problem behaviors among homeless and runaway youth. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79(1), 39–50.

 

[2] Evidence Against Bowlby and Conclusion to the Attachment Debate http://www.aqabpsychology.co.uk/?q=node/29

[3] ‘Patterns of Attachment’,  1978. Parent-child dyad, ie pairing  [b 1913 – d 1999]

[4] Winnicott, 1951 essay “Transitional Objects and Transitional Phenomena,”  Transactions that constitute love between two imperfect people. The imperfectlyattentive mother who does a better job than the “perfect” one who risks stifling her child’s development as a separate being.

[5] Bowlby’s famous 1952 study for WHO (World Health Organisation). Gilder’s withering appraisal of state provided child care by referring to by Bowlby’s studies (see Maternal Care and Mental Health and Deprivation of Maternal Care (New York, 1966).

[6] An American developmental psychologist known for her work in early emotional attachment.

[7] ‘Infants without Families’, by Anna Freud and Dorothy Burlingham, 1943.

[8] News-Medical in Child Health News reports, 25 May 2004, http://www.news-medical.net/?id=_87

[9] Aichhorn was an advocate of the idea that there was a distinction between manifest and latent delinquency, and believed that arrested development in youth was a precursor to antisocial behaviour. He also believed that this situation was caused by disturbances in early child-parent relationships.

[10] See also “Abolition of Marriage”, Gallagher, p 36-37.

[12] Children who spend time with their fathers have a higher IQ, The Telegraph, Sept 2008. http://www.telegraph.co.uk/education/3110360/Children-who-spend-time-with-their-fathers-have-a-higher-IQ.html .See also the journal, Evolution and Human Behaviour

[13] Lord Falconer

[17] “Cafcass: How we screen 200 private law cases a day” Community Care – Cafcass, 11 Jan 2012 http://www.communitycare.co.uk/Articles/11/01/2012/117918/Cafcass-How-we-screen-200-private-law-cases-a-day.htm

[18] See Bunny Banyai comments in the same article. ”I was almost psychotic with tiredness and so my relationship with my daughter suffered,” she said. ”She was pining for her dad.” http://www.smh.com.au/lifestyle/life/shared-custody-a-mistake-for-the-under2s-say-guidelines-20111214-1ouy6.html#ixzz1o51T8NYI

[19] “The effects of parental conflicts and divorce on the average well-being of pupils in secondary education.” by Jaap Dronkers, 1996.